Longitudinal changes in immunisation attitude trends have been assessed at population level previously in the UK [48] and using brief evidence-based tools regular ‘monitoring’ at local or national level, to AZD9291 chemical structure facilitate quick identification of and response
to problems, is now viable [49]. In addition to these previously untapped influences on parent’s decisions, substantial corroboration with the existing literature [10], [15], [41], [50], [51], [52], [53] and [54] was found, underscoring the importance of key factors including beliefs about disease and vaccine reaction likelihood and severity, trust in personal health professionals and the information they provide, perceptions of the wider policy and research context of the options available, and expectations of how friends and family will evaluate your decision. The organic emergence here of omission bias and excessive focus on regret indicates an ecological validity to effects previously seen mainly in experimental work [55], [56], [57] and [58]. This study has a number of methodological strengths. Analytic biases were countered
through member checking and coding by two analysts, MMR1 uptake was assessed objectively, and decision-making data were collected prospectively. Participants were recruited from a range of sources in order to obtain views broadly representative of each different parent decision group rather than of
‘activist’ groups, language support and two interview formats (face-to-face selleck compound and telephone) were used to facilitate and encourage participation parents who may have otherwise been excluded or excluded themselves, and collecting data from parents across the MMR1 decision spectrum facilitated Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II comparison within and between groups. However, the study is not without limitations. As enaction of a decision to postpone or refuse a vaccine has no objective marker – in contrast with enaction of a decision to accept a vaccine, which is clearly marked by receipt of the vaccine – arguably interviews with some parents in these groups could be considered retrospective. Biases were countered as described during the data coding stage, but interpretation was completed largely by one analyst (with informal discussion with the second analyst), so bias may have remained at this stage [59]. Data may have been coloured by their collection methods, for example the interviewer may have given non-verbal cues in face-to-face interviews which were not present in telephone interviews (however there was no systematic difference in interview format by decision group so between-group comparisons should remain valid), and the interpreter used with one participant may not have provided word-for-word translation (though they were asked explicitly to do this).